by Douglas Messerli
Joel Gold and Ian Gold Suspicious
Minds: How Culture Shapes Madness (New York: Free Press, 2014)
While working at the Bellevue Hospital psychiatric emergency
room in his first month as a doctor, Joel Gold encountered a seemingly mellow
and laid back individual—dressed in a “tie-dyed shirt, cargo shorts, and flip
flops,” his long hair held back in a bandana—who seemed to be suffering from a
strange delusion:
Albert believed that he was the subject of a TV show
or movie. In describing his experience, he compared it
to the acclaimed 1998 film The
Truman Show…directed
by Peter Weir. The protagonist, Truman Burbank…is
being watched by the whole world. Adopted in utero by
a television corporation, Truman lives with every moment
of his life being captured by thousands of cameras located
around Seahaven, the island community he never leaves
until his climatic escape to the “real” world.
The patient
“believed that all of the significant people in his life were involved in a
conspiracy to keep him on the air and in the dark about his own show.” At first
this strange delusion did not register with Gold as anything but yet another
variation of several delusions as outlined by the American Psychiatric
Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders, now, its fifth edition, described as DSM-5,
which recognizes numerous categories of disorder: “of development, mood,
trauma, sex and gender, sleep, drug abuse, and personality, among others.
Over a number of
years, however, Gold, discussing the various delusions with his brother, Ian,
who is Associate Professor of Philosophy and Psychiatry at McGill University,
began to see the “Truman” delusion as a series of delusions which a substantial
number of his patients had developed. And working with his brother, Gold began
to catalogue and explore what they called “The Truman Show Delusion,”
culminating in their book Suspicious
Minds: How Culture Shapes Madness.
This book,
however, uses this delusion—limited, apparently, to individuals who have seen
or heard about a popular motion picture—as merely the starting point for the
Golds’ exploration of contemporary psychiatry and the problems facing doctors
and patients. The field that once focused, given Freud’s influence, on personal
discussions with the patient and analysis of problems, has shifted increasingly
over the last few decades to become an issue of neuroscience, with drugs and
other mind-altering techniques. As the Golds point out, Freud’s activities
generally did not to delusions, which he felt were incurable.
Moreover, what we
quickly discern through the writers’ rational explanations of delusions and
their possible causes, is that psychiatry and its related activities has yet
developed into a true science in which one can explain causes and effects. A
great deal of what we know about the mind (and even its physical manifestation,
the brain) is purely speculative. While the American Psychiatric Association
has been able to create a kind of “check-list” delusions, doctors have still no
certainty as to how these delusions come about or, even more interestingly, why
there are so few delusions when one might imagine that human beings might
suffer from several thousand of such abnormal viewpoints. Yet nearly all
delusions, no matter how they differ within the specifics, can be said to
involve patterns that fall into these few categories.
Suspicious Minds is not only
comprehensible by the layman, but the Golds allow the reader to
speculate—through the several various theories they recount of how the brain of
sufferers becomes damaged—why. At the
same time, Joel Gold shares several cases with us, both historical and current,
helping us to at least to explore various theories, and even imaginatively
listen in on their patients’ dilemmas. Through chapters titled “A Short History
of Madness,” “One Hundred Years of Delusion,” “Hell Is Other People,” “Belief
Unhinged,” and “Beyond Belief,” the brothers take us through a thumbnail
history of the psychiatric profession then and now, detailing the shifts in
theories, and outlining new avenues of thought that have recently arisen.
By allowing us to
get a sense of the shape of this field of a future science, the Golds help
confirm their theories that not only are the specifics of delusions subject to
cultural and social changes, but that delusions themselves arise from our
encounters with others, deriving from our need as a species to evaluate our
social positioning within our worlds and perceive possible dangers others
represent such through deception, jealousy, and numerous other behavioral
patterns. Slowly and carefully, the Golds lay out evidence that delusional
behavior, and, in particular, schizophrenia, is higher in urban areas and
within particular groupings of individuals who have suffered childhood abuse,
immigrant adversity, social inferiority, and other forms of stress that
heighten our inbuilt “suspicion system” we have developed as a species.
Although the
Golds are perfectly willing to admit that some of these forces may physically
alter the brain through, for example, an increase or decrease of dopamine and
through other chemical substances created within the body, as well as admitting
that certain drugs clearly do alter the behavior of their patients for the
better, the brothers also imply that a newer kind psychiatry, far less passive
than Freudian analysis, for example, does profoundly effect and sometimes cures
their patients.
Moreover, the
Golds point out that what we might describe as “insane” behavior is often based
on seemingly sane solutions to the appearance of danger that have simply gone
awry. Delusional patients, they explain, are not much different from healthy
individuals except for their delusions from which, with supportive
conversations and drugs, patients can
be dissuaded. If contemporary “aspirational psychiatry” has, as the Golds
write, “hitched its wagon to the star of neuroscience,” they feel that, until
we know more specifics of the mind, that psychiatry should open itself to a
broad range of therapy. Quite convincingly, it seems to me, the Golds,
recognizing that delusions shift as our cultural perspectives change, suggest
that biology alone cannot take account of mental illness.
In a world in
which most of us realize that we are being watched by others, locally,
nationally, and internationally, is it a delusion to be slightly paranoid.
Perhaps those suffering from a delusion such as “The Truman Show” syndrome are
not expressing delusions of grandeur (which seem to be implied by the patients’
feeling that they are being “watched” by everyone) as much as they are
attempting to explain their fears that arise in a society where the NSA is capturing
nearly every message of their social communications, and where computer
communities such as Facebook and Twitter suddenly create complex and sometimes
dangerous urban-like groupings that once might have been experienced only in
large cities.
If suspicion
can become the source of a dangerous delusion, it can also help to protect us
from even more damaging encounters with others in our increasingly global
community whom we must daily face. A wise and suspicious e-mail user knows that
a letter from an unknown individual who claims he is willing to send millions
of dollars to the recipient if only he provides his bank account number,
providently deletes the message without answering. But when suspicion begins to
inform all of our email encounters it changes the way in which an individual
thinks, and surely will alter his or her ability to daily communicate with
others. How do most of us “draw that line” between healthy suspicion and a mind
infected by general mistrust?
The Golds hint
that perhaps there is no one “point” at which one has crossed the invisible
“line,” between sanity and insanity; just as importantly, they seemingly argue
that psychiatry does yet understand why some individuals more easily enter a
zone of fear and doubt that puts them into jeopardy. When clinical neuroscience
becomes the entire structure through which one approaches mental illness, the
Golds argue, it is like looking though a keyhole. Such a narrow viewpoint
“misses the wide biological perspective in which living things, humans
included, are best understood. The problem with aspirational psychiatry, then,
is not that it is too biological. The problem is that it is not biological
enough.” Environment and the social
world around us, in short, help us to explain not only what delusions are, but why we
develop them, and how we can work to
resolve them.
Los Angeles,
January 1, 2015
Reprinted from Rain
Taxi (Winter 2015)
No comments:
Post a Comment